Κριτικές του άρθρου |
Writing, reading
and graphic-motor deficits in Greek children with developmental dyslexia
The dyslexia studies examine the process of
writing in children in order to explore the causal factors leading to dyslexia,
the clarification of symptoms occurring in children and the attendant disorders
that affect their course in school. The study results proclaim the existence of
a close relationship between the symptoms of dyslexia and the abilities of
processing input, processing of language, senses, perception, attention,
memory, as well as the development of speech in pre-school years.
The present study aims at locating the errors in written texts that constitute
the diagnostic criteria for the classification of dyslexia in 3 categories. The
categorization of groups leads to the location of the respective types of
dyslexia.
The investigation uses the following methods: dictation of text, copy of text and free writing of text by description of a subject picture. To identify the types of errors in writing are generally studied 137 children with dyslexia from 2nd and 3rd grade who attend regular schools. For implementation of the experiment are used texts from the school textbooks for the 2nd and 3rd grade. Identification criteria are the errors of children admitted in writing.
The categorization of errors in groups
leads to the location of the respective types of dyslexia. The errors in text
writing can be categorized in groups, according to the disorder in the
acoustic-cognitive, visual-spatial or linguistic process of information. These
errors are associated with children’s gender and reading abilities; although,
the errors in text writing seem to relate to problems in text reading, bad handwriting and the ability to use lineal limits. These four abilities (writing, reading, handwriting
and lineal skills) constitute relevant elements of the written language
pathology that appears in children with developmental dyslexia.
Οι μελέτες σχετικά με την
δυσλεξία ερευνούν την διαδικασία της γραφής σε παιδιά προκειμένου να διερευνηθούν
οι αιτιακοί παράγοντες που οδηγούν στην δυσλεξία, την αποσαφήνιση των συμπτωμάτων
που εμφανίζονται στα παιδιά και τις συνοδές διαταραχές που επηρεάζουν την πορεία
τους στο σχολείο. Τα αποτελέσματα των ερευνών καταδεικνύουν την ύπαρξη στενής σχέσης
μεταξύ των συμπτωμάτων της δυσλεξίας και των ικανοτήτων επεξεργασίας των εισερχόμενων
πληροφοριών, γλωσσικής επεξεργασίας, αισθήσεων, αντίληψης, μνήμης, καθώς και
την εξέλιξη του προφορικού λόγου στην παιδική ηλικία.
Η παρούσα μελέτη στοχεύει
στον εντοπισμό των λαθών κατά την γραφή κειμένου που αποτελούν διαγνωστικά
κριτήρια για την ταξινόμηση της δυσλεξίας σε 3 κατηγορίες. Η κατηγοριοποίηση σε
ομάδες οδηγεί στον εντοπισμό των αντίστοιχων τύπων δυσλεξίας.
Για την έρευνα έχουν
χρησιμοποιηθεί οι εξής μέθοδοι: υπαγόρευση κειμένου, αντιγραφή κειμένου και
περιγραφή σύνθετης εικόνας. Για την ταυτοποίηση των λαθών γραφής εξετάστηκαν
137 παιδιά με δυσλεξία 2ης και 3ης τάξης που
παρακολουθούν κανονικό σχολείο. Για την εκτέλεση της έρευνας χρησιμοποιήθηκαν κείμενα
από τα σχολικά βιβλία της 2ης και 3ης τάξης. Τα κριτήρια
πιστοποίησης των λαθών είναι τα λάθη που παρουσιάζονται στην γραφή.
Η κατηγοριοποίηση των λαθών
σε ομάδες οδηγεί στον εντοπισμό των αντίστοιχων τύπων δυσλεξίας. τα λάθη στην
γραφή κειμένου μπορούν να κατηγοριοποιηθούν ανάλογα με την διαταραχή στην
ακουστική-γνωστική, οπτικο-χωρική και γλωσσική επεξεργασία των πληροφοριών. Τα
λάθη αυτά σχετίζονται με το φύλο των παιδιών και τις αναγνωστικές τους
ικανότητες. Όμως, τα λάθη κατά την γραφή κειμένου φαίνεται ότι σχετίζονται με
τα προβλήματα ανάγνωσης, τον γραφικό χαρακτήρα και τις γραφοκινητικές
δεξιότητες των παιδιών. Αυτές οι τέσσερεις ικανότητες αποτελούν σημαντικούς
παράγοντες για την παθολογία του γραπτού λόγου στα παιδιά με εξελικτική
δυσλεξία.
The dyslexia has been an object of study for the global scientific community for more than a century. Current studies examine the process of writing in children in order to explore the causal factors leading to dyslexia, the clarification of symptoms occurring in children and the attendant disorders that affect the course of children in school. The study results proclaim the existence of a close relationship between the symptoms of developmental dyslexia and the abilities of processing input, processing of language, senses, perception, attention, memory, as well as the development of speech in pre-school years.
According to Boder (1), “it may be said that dyslexic children are found to be consistently more dysorthographic than dyslexic; the three reading-spelling patterns reveal that most of the errors made by dyslexic children do not occur at random, but in patterns of errors; identification of the three reading-spelling patterns helps to clarify why the classic dyslexic errors do not distinguish subtypes among dyslexic children and even tend to obscure their heterogeneity; all of the classic errors, notably the static and kinetic reversals, have been observed in all three of our subtypes; it would appear, therefore, that although the classic errors are valuable diagnostic signs, especially significant in older children, they are not invariable concomitants of developmental dyslexia; aside from the classic reversals, most of the dyslexic errors fall into two main groups-the intelligible phonetic errors and the unintelligible dysphonetic errors” (1).
According to, “it may be said that dyslexic children are found to be consistently more dysorthographic than dyslexic; the three reading-spelling patterns reveal that most of the errors made by dyslexic children do not occur at random, but in patterns of errors; identification of the three reading-spelling patterns helps to clarify why the classic dyslexic errors do not distinguish subtypes among dyslexic children and even tend to obscure their heterogeneity; all of the classic errors, notably the static and kinetic reversals, have been observed in all three of our subtypes; it would appear, therefore, that although the classic errors are valuable diagnostic signs, especially significant in older children, they are not invariable concomitants of developmental dyslexia; aside from the classic reversals, most of the dyslexic errors fall into two main groups-the intelligible phonetic errors and the unintelligible dysphonetic errors”.
Based on current studies on developmental dyslexia, the errors of children’s writing and reading a text are classified in three categories:1) acoustic errors (errors due to deficit in acoustic-cognitive processing of information), 2) visual errors (errors due to deficit in visual-spatial processing of information) and 3) linguistic errors (errors in linguistic processing of information). A review of studies helps us understand the findings lead to the conclusion of a complex etiology and pathology not only in children but in adults with dyslexia as well.
As Ziegler illustrates (2) (2006) “it has been commonly agreed that developmental dyslexia in different languages has a common biological origin: a dysfunction of left posterior temporal brain regions dealing with phonological processes; it seems quite clear that children in all languages need to develop appropriate symbol–sound mapping for learning to read; in alphabetic languages, children learn to map letters onto phonemes; in logographic languages, however, children might need to learn the mapping of complex graphic-motor programs onto whole-word phonology; to perform these mappings, the brain uses different neural circuits depending on the grain size and transparency with which writing systems represent phonology; at a cognitive level, however, impaired phonology is still the major cause of dyslexia”. Gunnell & Parlow (3)associate “poor interhemispheric communication with everyday memory problems and poor phonological processing with poor working memory”. Caravolas, Hulme, & Snowling (4) accept that “skilled spelling requires a foundation in phonological transcoding ability which in turn enables the formation of orthographic representations”. Other theories illustrate that “poor comprehenders despite having adequate phonological decoding skills, have problems reading words that are typically read with support from semantics” (5). A study by Blomert & Mitterer (6) is compatible with the phonological-coding deficit theory. According to this theory “the deficit is in the phonetic transform from analogue neural response patterns to digital lexical/phonological representations”. According to Lavidor, Johnston, & Snowling (7) “explaining the between-hemisphere differences in the relative engagement and specialization of the different representations, enhanced sensitivity to orthographic cues is developed in some cases of dyslexia when a normal, phonology-based left hemisphere word recognition processing is not achieved”. Ziegler, Perry, Ma-Wyatt, Ladner, & Schulte-Korne (8) claim that “dyslexics exhibited a reading speed deficit, a specific non-word reading deficit, and a phonological decoding mechanism that operates extremely slowly and serially”. Also Tremblay, Monetta, & Joanette (9) “complexity processing modulates the hemispheric dynamic associated with phonology and semantics differently”.
Results from investigation of Zaidel & Petters (10) “help clarify an outstanding debate about whether phonological recoding is necessary for reading either words or sentences; right hemispheres reading of single words without phonological recoding is neurologically possible and the absence of phonological recoding may reflect constraints in short-term verbal memories and, in turn, restrict them to reading phrases shorter than three words long; together, this suggests that phonetic recoding is necessary for sentence reading”.
Research of
Beaton (11) indicates that “some dyslexic
children and adults have problems in subtle aspects of speech perception or
discrimination which might underpin their phonological problems”. According to Alain, et al. (12)
(2005) “deficits in parsing acoustic events may contribute to speech
perception problems often observed in older adults and in individuals with
dyslexia proposed that the explicit registration and discrimination of acoustic
objects require computations subsequent to the initial processing of sounds in
the ascending pathway and primary acoustic cortex, and suggest that these
computations might be carried out in the planum temporal; according to this
model, neural activity in the planum temporale should vary with listeners’
likelihood of hearing one or two acoustic objects”. Boets, Wouters, van Wieringen, & Ghesquiere (13) argue that “there is most evidence to
situate the core of the reading and spelling problem at the level of
higher-order phonological processing; low-level acoustic and speech perception
problems are relatively over-represented in the group of literacy-impaired
subjects and they might possibly aggravate the phonological and literacy
problem”. Study of Bogliotti, Serniclaes,
Messaoud-Galusi, & Sprenger-Charolles (14) confirmed “the relationship between reading skills and speech
perception; examination of individual performances showed that both the deficit
in categorical perception and the concomitant increase in allophonic
sensitivity were fairly prevalent among children affected by dyslexia”. Study
of Brunellière, Dufour, Nguyen, &
Frauenfelder (15) shows that “the
regional variability in the speech input to which listeners are exposed affects
the perception of speech sounds in their own accent”. Gerrits and de Bree (16) find “lower speech perception and
production performance in children at familial risk of dyslexia; perceptual
discrimination between speech sounds belonging to different phoneme categories
is better than that between sounds falling within the same category; this
property, known as “categorical perception”, is weaker in children affected by
dyslexia; dyslexic children use an allophonic mode of speech perception that,
although without straightforward consequences for oral communication, has
obvious implications for the acquisition of alphabetic writing. According to Serniclaes, Van Heghe, Mousty, Carre, &
Sprenger-Charolles (17) “allophonic
perception specifically affects the mapping between graphemes and phonemes,
contrary to other manifestations of dyslexia, and may be a core deficit”.
According to Snowling (18) “at about the same time as the first epidemiological studies were being conducted, cognitive psychologists began comparing groups of normal and dyslexic readers in a range of experimental paradigms; these researchers pursued the then popular idea that dyslexia was a perceptual deficit and studies investigated visual perception, visual memory, cross-modal transfer between visual and verbal codes and perceptual learning, and other skills in relation to reading ability”. Facoetti & Molteni (19) argues that “the deficit of the mechanism subserving selection of stimuli (spatial attention) in the right visual field might determine some visual perceptual disorders that are frequently found in subjects with specific reading disorder or dyslexia”. Mycroft, Behrmann, & Kay (20) claims that “a longstanding and controversial issue concerns the underlying mechanisms that give rise to letter-by letter reading: while some researchers propose a prelexical, perceptual basis for the disorder, others postulate a postlexical, linguistic source for the problem; findings are consistent with significant visuoperceptual impairment in letter-by letter reading that adversely affects reading performance as well as performance on other non-reading tasks”. Lorusso, Facoetti, Pesenti, Cattaneo, Molteni, & Geiger (21) support that “the wide distribution of recognition, similar across the various subtypes of dyslexia, suggests a general characteristic of visual perception, and possibly a different visual-attentional mode”.
Snowling (18) accepts that “there is evidence that “dyslexic children have trouble with long-term verbal learning; this problem may account for many classroom difficulties, including problems memorizing the days of the week or the months of the year, mastering multiplication tables, and learning a foreign language; in a similar vein, this problem may be responsible for the poor vocabulary development often observed in dyslexic children”. Barber & Kutas (22) support that “during visual word recognition, the brain extracts various types of information presumed to be characterizing word representations; to that end, the visual (or word) processing system could segment words into a variety of sublexical units, associated with different information types (phonological, syllabic, morphological)”. Boder (1) has proposed that “one can distinguish dysphonetic dyslexics, whose deficiencies lie essentially in deciphering skills, dyseidetic ones who are mainly deficient in visual recognition and mixed cases combining the two deficiencies” (23).
Beaton (11) claims that “there is certainly a genetic connection between language lateralization and handedness although the exact nature of the relationship is controversial and handedness is probably related in some way to reading and dyslexia”.
Nicolson, Fawcett, & Dean (24) support that “disorders of cerebellar development can in fact cause the impairments in reading and writing characteristics of dyslexia, a view consistent with the recently appreciated role of the cerebellum in language-related skills”.
According to
Beaton (11) “testosterone has been linked
to the size of another brain structure which some have seen as having a role to
play in dyslexic symptomatology; this structure is the corpus callosum”. Barber & Kutas (22) argues that “reading is not a genetically transmitted cognitive
ability; it seems to involve the progressive specialization of specific brain
areas; it has thus been possible to associate activity in different brain
regions during reading with initial perceptual analysis, orthographic decoding,
and phonological processing”. Study of Corbetta (25)
“elucidates the pathophysiology of clinical brain disorders, that
involve attentional and visual perceptual deficits, e.g. unilateral neglect,
attentional deficit disorders, or dyslexia”. According to Mano, Osmon, & Klein (26) “the morphological factor could be
interpreted as a construct representing perceptual decomposition of the word
stem from prefix and suffix morphemes; their results add to the growing
literature supporting the double-deficit hypothesis of dyslexia and have
clinical implications for assessment of the orthographic aspects of dyslexia”. Baillieux, Vandervliet, Manto, Parizel, De Deyn, &
Mariën (27) propose “a new
hypothesis regarding the pathophysiological mechanisms of developmental
dyslexia; given the sites of activation in the cerebellum in the dyslexic
group, a defect of the intra-cerebellar distribution of activity is suspected,
suggesting a disorder of the processing or transfer of information within the
cerebellar cortex. According to Schulz, et al.
(28) “developmental dyslexia is a
specific disorder of reading acquisition characterized by a phonological core
deficit; sentence reading is also impaired in dyslexic readers, but whether
semantic processing deficits contribute is unclear; sentence reading was
characterized by activation in a left-lateralized language network; semantic
processing was characterized by activation in left-hemispheric regions of the
inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex”. Results from a study of
Steinbrink et al. (29), “based on a
combined analysis of white and gray matter abnormalities, provide exceedingly
strong evidence for a disconnection syndrome or dysfunction of cortical areas
relevant for reading and spelling; this imbalance of neuronal communication
between the respective brain areas might be the crucial point for the
development of dyslexia”. According to Rae, et
al. (30) “the relationship of cerebellar asymmetry to phonological
decoding ability and handedness, together with previous finding of altered metabolite
ratios in the cerebellum of dyslexics, suggest that there are alterations in
the neurological organization of the cerebellum which relate to phonological
decoding skills, in addition to motor skills and handedness”.
The present study aims at locating the errors in written texts that constitute the diagnostic criteria for the classification of dyslexia in 3 different categories: acoustic, visual and linguistic dyslexia. The categorization of errors in groups of acoustic, visual and linguistic errors leads to the location of the respective types of dyslexia, which are analyzed according to the gender and grade of the children and their specific characteristics such as reading abilities and handwriting.
The investigation uses the following methods: dictation of text, copy of text and free writing of text by description of a subject picture.
To identify the
types of errors in writing are generally studied 137 children with dyslexia
from 2nd and 3rd grade who attend regular schools, 77 of
which are boys and 60 girls, 72 children in 2nd and
For implementation of the experiment are used texts from the school textbooks for the 2nd and 3rd grade. Periods of the experiment were chosen so that the texts have already been taught to the children.
The first text is given to the child for dictation with the instruction to write the text that will be dictated by the examiner. Examiner dictates every time no more than 3-4 words. Second text is given to the child in printed form with the instruction to copy it. For the description a black and white picture is selected, which depicts a playground and the child is given the instruction to write what he/she sees.
The time of the study was set at 15 minutes for each text, but some of the children needed more time for the copy from dictation or description of the picture, so the examination time differs from child to child.
Identification criteria are the errors of children admitted in writing. Written texts are checked and the errors that have been described in each table are labeled in a certain way. Errors are grouped according to their individual characteristics, based on sound, visual or linguistic deficiency etiology.
In this particular experiment, therefore, that the data follow a regular distribution for the analysis used the distribution of χ2 (non parametric statistics). Errors of children are classified each into 2, 3 or more categories according to a property (Robert & James, 1960). For implementation of this analysis the number of errors that children made for different types of text are collected and analyzed by gender, class and age of children during the study. For gender analysis a random number of 54 boys and girls is used. For education level analysis a random number of 60 children in 2nd and 60 children in 3rd grade is used. For reading ability level analysis a random number of 50 children with reading problems and 50 children without reading problems is used. For handwriting level analysis a random number of 50 children with bad handwriting and 50 children with good handwriting is used. For linear restrictions’ skills level analysis a random number of 49 children with deficits in linear restrictions’ skills and 49 children without deficits in linear restrictions’ skills is used. Children who have no data in the three texts are not included in the analysis.
For further
investigation about the strength of the associations between means of the children’s
errors in writing and children’s gender, grade, age, reading problems and
handwriting SPSS one-way ANOVA statistics are used.
The experiment
includes data from the texts of 137 children in 2nd and 3rd
grade. From total 77 boys and 60 girls, 72 boys and 55 girls have valid cases
for acoustic errors (AE), 42 boys and 25 girls have valid cases for visual
errors (VE) and 76 boys and 57 girls have valid cases for linguistic errors
(LE). For AE the boys’ mean is 10.28 and the girls’ 7.13; for VE the boys’ mean
is 2.76 and the girls’ 2.76; for LE the boys’ mean is 13.63 and the girls’
9.07. From total 72 2nd grade children and 65 3rd grade
children, 66 2-grade and 61 3-grade have valid cases for AE, 39 2-grade and 28
3-grade have valid cases for VE and 69 2-grade and 64 3-grade have valid cases
for LE. For AE the 2-grade mean is 9.42 and the 3-grade 8.36; for VE the
2-grade mean is 3.21 and the 3-grade 2.14; for LE the 2-grade mean is 10.86 and
the 3-grade 12.56. From total 57 children with reading problems (RP) and 80
without RP, 56 children with RP and 71 without have valid cases for AE, 31
children with RP and 36 children without have valid cases for VE and 57
children with RP and 76 children without have valid cases for LE. For AE the RP
mean is 12.41 and the RP-not 6.15; for VE the RP mean is 3.32 and the RP-not
2.28; for LE the RP mean is 15.68 and RP-not 8.67. From total 96 children with
bad handwriting (HW) and 41 children with good HW, 94 children with bad HW and
33 with good HW have valid cases for AE, 54 children with bad HW and 13 with
good HW have valid cases for VE and 95 children with bad HW and 38 with good HW
have valid cases for LE. For AE the bad HW mean is 10.29 and the good HW 5.00;
for VE the bad HW mean is 2.59 and the good HW 3.46; for LE the bad HW mean is
13.49 and the good HW 7.13. From total 82 children with deficits in linear
restrictions’ skills (LRS) and 55 without LRS, 80 children with LRS and 47
without have valid cases for AE, 46 children with LRS and 21 children without
have valid cases for VE and 81 children with LRS and 52 without have valid
cases for LE. For AE the LRS mean is 10.86 and the LRS-not 5.60; for VE the LRS
mean is 2.63 and the LRS-not 3.05; for LE the LRS mean is 14.25 and the LRS-not
7.67.
From total 137 children, 127 have valid cases for AE, 67 have valid cases for VE and 133 have valid cases for LE. AE are acoustic changes of letters, omission of letters, syllables or words, metathesis and antimetathesis of letters and syllables, errors in letter writing [j] and addition of letters, syllables or words. VE are visual changes of letters, repetition of letters, syllables or words, mirroring of letters, syllables or letter sequences, omission and repetitions of sequences. LE are errors in syntax and language usage, union and disunion of words, errors in use of punctuation marks and capital letters. Others errors (OE) in writing of text are mixed acoustic-visual changes of letters, grammatical-orthographical errors and errors in use of stresses.
95 children
have valid cases for acoustic changes of letters (M=4.25), 104 children have valid cases
for omission
of letters, syllables or words (M=4.84), 37 children have valid cases for metathesis and
antimetathesis of letters and syllables (M=1.84), 56 children have valid cases
for addition
of letters, syllables or words (M=2.00) and 34 children have valid cases for errors in letter
writing [j] (M=1.32). 42 children have valid cases
for visual
changes of letters (M=2.21),
20 children have valid cases for repetition of letters, syllables or words (M=1.50), 29 children
have valid cases for mirroring of letters, syllables or letter sequences (M=1.79), 9 children have
valid cases for omission and repetitions of sequences
(M=1.11). 90 children have valid
cases for errors in syntax and language usage (M=2.60), 88 children
have valid cases for union and disunion of words (M=6.40), 94
children have valid cases for errors in use of punctuation marks (M=4.32) and 79 children
have valid cases for errors in use of capital letters (M=3.49). 41 children have valid cases
for mixed acoustic-visual changes of letters (M=1.83), 131 children have valid cases for
grammatical-orthographical errors (M=25.20)
and 123 children have valid cases for errors in use of stresses (M=26.26).
In order to explore the relationship between the sum of the different types of errors in writing and children’s gender a distribution of χ2 was conducted. Statistical analysis of acoustic, visual and linguistic errors in the writing of text depending on the gender of the children shows that there are statistically significant differences (χ2=6.65, df=2, N=54, p=.038), so that gender affects the types of errors in writing of text, and specifically the acoustic errors, with the boys to do most of them. Differences in boys and girls are significant and the boys have approximately 2 times more errors when writing a text than the girls. Visual and acoustic errors of boys have the same proportion. In writing of text the boys are expected to make many more errors. The acoustic errors have biggest frequency, which is almost as visual and linguistic errors together. However, this is not depending of the gender, because the sum of visual and linguistic errors in the girls was close to the number of acoustic errors, something which appears in boys too.
In order to
explore the relationship between the sum of the different types of errors in
writing and children’s grade a distribution of χ2 was conducted. Statistical analysis of acoustic, visual and
linguistic errors in the writing of text depending on the education level of
children shows that there are no statistically significant differences (χ2=1.80, df=2, N=60, p=.405), so that the education level
does not affect the types of errors in writing of text by the children. Errors
of children at 2nd and 3rd grade do not have significant
differences. Children while writing a text are expected to make the same
mistakes as children in 2nd and 3rd grade. The sum of
errors changes very little during increase of the level of education.
Table 1
In order to
explore the relationship between the sum of the different types of errors in
writing and children’s reading ability a distribution of χ2 was conducted. Statistical analysis
of acoustic, visual and linguistic errors in the writing of the readable text,
depending on the reading skills shows that there are statistically significant
differences (χ2=11.60, df=2, N=50, p=.003), so that the reading ability of children affects the types
of errors when writing a text, and specifically the
acoustic errors, with the children who have problems with reading doing most of
them. Children who have trouble in reading made more
errors when writing text, in the three categories. Most errors that children
make are acoustic and visual have the lowest sum. This means that in children
who have trouble in reading, the deficit prevails in acoustic processing of
linguistic information, which leads to the emergence of acoustic errors when
writing text. The relationship between writing and reading shows the difficulty
of separating these processes, one affects the other. Ability of reading text
determines the success of the children in writing.
In order to
explore the relationship between the sum of the different types of errors in
writing and children’s handwriting skills a distribution of χ2 was conducted. Statistical analysis of acoustic,
visual and linguistic errors in the writing of the text depending on the
handwriting of children shows that there are statistically significant
differences (χ2=10.58, df=2, N=50, p=.005), so
that the handwriting of children affects the types of errors when writing text,
and specifically linguistic errors, with the children who have ugly handwriting
to make most of them. The children’s skills of writing letters
associated with the occurrence of different types of errors when writing text.
Children who have ugly handwriting made more errors in writing. These skills
are abilities developed from pre-aged in drawing and construction tasks in
nursery school. When children start school they learn the forms of letters and
how to "draw" them. The child who does not write the correct form of
letters meets major difficulties in the differentiation and storing letters,
and other mental abilities that are associated with the proper knowledge of
letters. On the other hand, teachers often say that children, who have problems
with reading and writing, knowingly write a letter to another to mask their
inability to write properly. However, it cannot be confirmed by the data of
this experiment.
In order to explore the relationship between the sum of the different types of errors in writing and children’s linear restrictions’ skills (LRS) a distribution of χ2 was conducted. Statistical analysis of acoustic, visual and linguistic errors in the writing of the text depending on the linear restrictions’ skills shows that there are statistically significant differences (χ2=15.18, df=2, N=49, p<.001), so that the appearance of distortion of words and lines affect the types of errors when writing text, and specifically acoustic errors, with children who do not comply with the restrictions make most of them. Poor ability to enforce restrictions on the line in the workbook of children led to an increase in the number of acoustic errors. These children have expressed in writing in the pathology of children who know how to write properly on the line.
Table 2
In order to explore the means of the different types of errors in writing among the gender groups a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s gender includes 2 grouping variables: boys and girls. Comparing the children’s gender groups on AE, VE and LE, no statistically significant difference was found among the AE (F(1,102)=3.211,p=.076), VE (F(1,51)=2.404,p=.127) and LE (F(1,108)=.959,p=.330). However, because the variances are unequal for VE, we are uncertain whether to trust results for VE. The boys’ mean for AE is 7.84, for VE 2.45 and LE 11.15. The girls’ means for AE is 5.70, VE 1.70 and LE 9.42. This indicates that the boys have a strongest pathology than the girls, but the differences are not big enough to understand the strength of those differences.
In order to explore the means of the different types of errors in writing among the grade groups a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s grade includes 2 grouping variables: 2nd grade and 3rd grade. Comparing the children’s grade groups on AE, VE and LE, no statistically significant difference was found among the AE (F(1,102)=.032,p=.858), VE (F(1,51)=2.633,p=.111) and LE (F(1,108)=.680,p=.411). However, because the variances are unequal for VE, we are uncertain whether to trust results for VE. Children in 2nd grade and children in 3rd grade have approximately equal means for AE, 2nd grade’s mean is 6.76 and 3rd grade’s mean is 6.98. The means for VE are 2.52 for 2nd grade and 1.75 for 3rd grade. The means for LE are 9.64 for 2nd grade and 11.09 for 3rd grade. This indicates that more children in 2nd grade make VE and more children in 3rd grade make LE, but the differences are not big enough to understand the strength of those differences.
In order to explore the means of the different types of errors in writing among the age groups a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s age includes 3 grouping variables: 7, 8 and 9 years old children. Comparing the children’s age groups on AE, VE and LE, no statistically significant difference was found among the AE (F(2,101)=1.144,p=.323), VE (F(2,50)=.370,p=.693) and LE (F(2,107)=.290,p=.749). The means for 7 years old children for AE is 5.94, for VE 2.12 and for LE 9.97. The means for 8 years old for AE is 6.78, for VE 2.36 and for LE 10.11. The means for 9 years old for AE is 8.52, for VE 1.83 and for LE 11.73. This indicates that the bigger children means are bigger than the smaller children, but the differences are not big enough to understand the strength of those differences.
In order to explore the differences between boys and girls in both grades and the means of the different types of errors in writing among the gender (by grade) groups a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s gender (by grade) includes 4 grouping variables: boys in 2nd grade, boys in 3rd grade, girls in 2nd grade and girls in 3rd grade. Comparing the children’s gender (by grade) groups on AE, VE and LE, no statistically significant difference was found among the AE (F(3,100)=1.145,p=.335), VE (F(3,49)=1.671,p=.185) and LE (F(3,106)=.637,p=.593). However, because the variances are unequal for VE, we are uncertain whether to trust results for VE. The means of boys in 2nd grade for AE is 7.45, for VE 2.84 and for LE 10.35. The means of boys in 3rd grade for AE is 8.31, for VE 1.93 and for LE 12.11. The means of girls in 2nd grade for AE is 5.70, for VE 1.90 and for LE 8.37. The means of girls in 3rd grade for AE is 5.70, for VE 1.50 and for LE 10.10. This indicates that boy have mean values higher than girls for AE, VE and LE. AE and LE have higher mean values in 3rd grade than 2nd grade, but VE have higher mean values in 2nd grade; although the differences are not big enough to understand the strength of those differences.
In order to
explore the differences between little and big children in both grades and the
means of the different types of errors in writing among the grade (by age) groups
a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s grade (by age) includes 4 grouping
variables: little children in 2nd grade, big children in 2nd
grade, little children in 3rd grade and big children in 3rd
grade. Comparing the children’s grade (by age) groups on AE, VE and LE, no
statistically significant difference was found among the AE (F(3,100)=1.278,p=.286), VE (F(3,49)=1.518,p=.222) and LE (F(3,106)=.314,p=.816).
However, because the variances are unequal for VE, we are uncertain whether to
trust results for VE. The mean for AE is 5.94 for little 2nd grade
children, 8.16 for big 2nd grade children, 5.97 for little 3rd
grade children and 8.52 for big 3rd grade children. The mean for VE
is 2.12 for little 2nd grade children, 3.00 for big 2nd
grade children, 1.67 for little 3rd grade children and 1.83 for big
3rd grade children. The mean for LE is 9.97 for little 2nd
grade children, 9.14 for big 2nd grade children, 10.69 for little 3rd
grade children and 11.73 for big 3rd grade children. This indicates
that little children in both grades have approximately equal means for AE; for
VE and LE the means for 2nd grade and 3rd grade are
approximately equal; although, the differences are not big enough to understand
the strength of those differences.
In order to
explore the means of the different types of errors in writing among the reading
problems (RP) groups a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s RP includes 2
grouping variables: children with RP and children without RP. Comparing the
children’s RP groups on AE, VE and LE, statistically significant difference was
found among the AE (F(1,102)=4.383,p=.039), VE (F(1,51)=7.657,p=.008) and
LE (F(1,108)=11.85,p=.001). However, because the variances
are unequal, we are uncertain whether to trust these results. The mean of AE is
8.35 for children with RP and 5.84 for children without RP. The mean of VE is
2.91 for children with RP and 1.65 for children without RP. The mean of LE is
13.91 for children with RP and 8.05 for children without RP. This indicates
that children with RP have bigger mean values than children without RP.
Table 3
In order to explore the differences between
major and minor RP and the means of the different types of errors in writing
among the RP (major or minor) groups a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s RP
(major or minor) includes 3 grouping variables: children with major RP,
children with minor RP and children without RP. Comparing the children’s RP
(major or minor) groups on AE, VE and LE, no statistically significant
difference was found among the AE (F(2,101)=2.550,p=.083), but statistically significant
difference was found among the VE (F(2,50)=4.347,p=.018) and LE (F(2,107)=5.839,p=.004).
However, because the variances are unequal, we are uncertain whether to trust
results. The means of children with major RP for AE is 8.62, for VE 2.29 and
for LE 15.19. The means of children with minor RP for AE is 8.32, for VE 3.12
and for LE 12.93. The means of children without RP for AE is 5.73, for VE 1.63
and for LE 8.08. This indicates that children with RP have mean values higher
than children without RP for AE, VE and LE. Children with major RP and children
with minor RP have approximately equal mean values in AE; children with major
RP have higher mean values than children with minor RP in LE; children with
minor RP have higher mean values than children with major RP for VE.
Games-Howell test indicates that there are no significant differences among
groups except the differences among children with major RP and children without
RP; in AE between children with major RP and children with minor RP MD=.304 (p=.986); between children with major RP and children without RP MD=2.892 (p=.192); between children with minor RP and children without RP MD=2.588 (p=.152); in VE between children with major RP and children with
minor RP MD=-.839 (p=.526); between children with major RP
and children without RP MD=.652 (p=.373); between children with minor RP
and children without RP MD=1.492 (p=.091); in LE between children with
major RP and children with minor RP MD=2.256
(p=.765); between children with major
RP and children without RP MD=7.111 (p=.033); between children with minor RP
and children without RP MD=4.854 (p=.098).
Table 4
In order to explore the means of the
different types of errors in writing among the handwriting (HW) groups a
one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s handwriting includes 2 grouping variables:
children with dab HW and children with good HW. Comparing the children’s HW
groups on AE, VE and LE, statistically significant difference was found among
the LE (F(1,108)=4.790,p=.031), but not on AE (F(1,102)=3.583,p=.061) and VE (F(1,51)=.562,p=.457). The mean AE is 7.57 for
children with bad HW and 5.07 for children with good HW; the mean VE is 2.26
for children with bad HW and 1.82 for children with good HW; the mean LE is
11.62 for children with bad HW and 7.52 for children with good HW. This
indicates that children with bad HW have bigger mean values than children
without bad HW.
Table 5
In order to explore the means of the different types of errors in writing among the LRS groups a one-way ANOVA was used. Children’s LRS includes 2 grouping variables: children with deficits in LRS and children without deficits in LRS. Comparing the children’s LRS groups on AE, VE and LE, statistically significant difference was found among the LE (F(1,108)=4.768,p=.031), but not on AE (F(1,102)=2.389,p=.125) and VE (F(1,51)=.116,p=.735). The mean AE is 7.62 for children with LRS and 5.73 for children with not LRS; the mean VE is 2.23 for children with good LRS and 2.06 for children with bad LRS; the mean LE is 11.95 for children with good LRS and 8.13 for children with bad LRS. This indicates that children with good LRS have bigger mean values than children with bad LRS.
Table 6
Acoustic, visual and linguistic errors in the writing of text represent categories of errors that are used to determine the forms of dyslexia. It is these errors to be identified as dyslexic. Therefore, they are analyzed in relation to gender and education level.
According to the definition of dyslexia, it is not the result of the absence of stimuli, sensory dysfunction and insufficient training and environmental factors, however, they can co-exist. Therefore, the symptoms of dyslexia (in this case errors when writing a text), are not a result of these causes. The education level and the quality of the training process are causal factors and dyslexic errors in writing of text are not their results. So, errors during writing of text to be identified as dyslexic must demonstrate that they have connection with the gender of children and have no connection with the level of education.
Analysis of different types of errors in relation with gender shows that there are significant differences between the sum of acoustic, visual and linguistic errors. Although, an analysis of AE, VE and LE means in gender, grade and age groups shows that there are not significant differences between groups. The boys have greater mean values than the girls, which indicate a strongest pathology in writing. However, the differences are not big enough for such a conclusion. The same result is valid for the grade and age analysis. In grade groups we see that the means for AE and LE are too close, while the VE mean for 2nd grade is a little higher than the 3rd grade’s mean. Combining gender and grade we see that there is no significant differences in AE, VE and LE means. Combining the grade and age also there is no significant differences between means.
Exploring the means of AE, VE and LE among RP groups, we see that children with RP have greater mean values than the children without RP. The strongest difference appears in LE and VE. By analysis the means of AE, VE and LE among HW and LRS groups we see that there are significant differences for LE, but not for AE and VE. This indicates that only LE strongly depend on the RP, HW and LRS. By separating the RP children group in 2 groups of children with major RP and children with minor RP and combining them with no-RP children’s group we see that VE and LE have significant differences among groups. AE means for children with major RP and children with minor RP are approximately equal. Although, the differences seems be located at the major RP and no-RP groups.
In conclusion we say that:
The errors in text writing of children of pre-school years can be categorized in groups of acoustic, visual and linguistic errors, according to their specific characteristics, which lead us to the categorization of the errors in acoustic, visual and linguistic errors, according to the disorder in the acoustic-cognitive, visual-spatial or linguistic process of incoming information.
Acoustic errors in writing of text are acoustic changes of letters, omission of letters, syllables or words, metathesis and antimetathesis of letters and syllables, errors in letter writing [j] and addition of letters, syllables or words. Visual errors in writing of text are visual changes of letters, repetition of letters, syllables or words, mirroring of letters, syllables or letter sequences, omission and repetitions of sequences. Linguistic errors in writing of text are errors in syntax and language usage, union and disunion of words, errors in use of punctuation marks and capital letters.
The errors in text writing seem to relate to problems in text reading, bad handwriting and the ability to use lineal limits. These four abilities (writing, reading, handwriting and lineal skills) constitute relevant elements of the written language pathology that appears in children with developmental dyslexia.
Further
research and analysis needs to be done in order to clarify the appearance of
different errors in text writing according to the type of text before any
categorization of the types of developmental dyslexia appearance. Further
research is necessary in order to discover a possible relation of the errors in
text writing with errors in text reading of children with developmental
dyslexia. We would expect that the children present the same errors in both
writing and reading, but this research cannot possibly provide such a
conclusion.
1. Boder, Elena. Developmental Dyslexia: a
Diagnostic Approach Based on Three Atypical Reading-spelling Patterns. Developmental
Medicine and Child Neurology. 8 1973, Vol. 15, 5, pp. 663-687.
http://www.scopus.com/scopus/inward/record.url?eid=2-s2.0-0015900561&partnerID=40.
2. Ziegler, Johannes C. Do differences in brain
activation challenge universal theories of dyslexia? Brain and Language. 9
2006, Vol. 98, 3, pp. 341–343. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WC0-4GP7P1X-1/2/0a7033b273d15051af027b95595dcebb.
3. Gunnell, Jessica G. and Parlow, Shelley E.
Interhemispheric communication, phonological processing, and memory in
dyslexia: An investigation of relationships among three domains. TENNET:
Theoretical and Experimental Neuropsychology - XVII Annual Meeting, June 21-23,
2007,
4. Caravolas, Markéta, Hulme, Charles and
Snowling, Margaret J. The Foundations of Spelling Ability: Evidence from a
3-Year Longitudinal Study. Journal of Memory and Language. 11 2001, Vol.
45, 4, pp. 751–774. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WK4-457VF1T-D/2/2e539cd5d709c6b391e0e779512948fc.
5. Nation, Kate and Snowling, Margaret J.
Semantic Processing and the Development of Word-Recognition Skills: Evidence
from Children with Reading Comprehension Difficulties. Journal of memory and
language. 7 1998, Vol. 39, 1, pp. 85–101.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WK4-45KV6HH-5/2/8cbc04e22a051e9fc051531c3835c858.
6. Blomert, Leo and Mitterer, Holger. The
fragile nature of the speech-perception deficit in dyslexia: Natural vs.
synthetic speech. Brain and Language. 4 2004, Vol. 89, 1, pp. 21–26.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WC0-49SFF4N-3/2/6444b0ebe927a573769f8427d1c5decf.
7. Lavidor, Michal, Johnston, Rhona and Snowling,
Margaret J. When phonology fails: Orthographic neighbourhood effects in
dyslexia. Brain and Language. 3 2006, Vol. 96, 3, pp. 318–329.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WC0-4GVGTC2-1/2/e37b6787b71e215c0e332b2461cb34ca.
8. Ziegler, Johannes C., et al. Developmental
dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? J.
Experimental Child Psychology. 11 2003, Vol. 86, 3, pp. 169–193.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WJ9-49S3C95-1/2/45a14664c3ca168b0105351f3165167a.
9. Tremblay, Tania, Monetta, Laura and Joanette,
Yves. Complexity and hemispheric abilities: Evidence for a differential
impact on semantics and phonology. Brain & Language. 2 2009, Vol.
108, 2, pp. 67–72.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WC0-4V4KC5P-1/2/e87a6827efe2188f72a9e53ea98e7ddb.
10. Zaidel, Eran and Petters, Ann M.
Phonological Encoding and ldeographic Reading by the Disconnected Right
Hemisphere: Two Case Studies. Brain and Language. 11 1981, Vol. 14, 2,
pp. 205-234. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WC0-4D6RF4T-34/2/f266b15f35777c1a85bf1320adedbaa1.
11. Beaton, Alan A. The Neurobiology of
Dyslexia. [book auth.] Martin Turner and John Rack. The Study of Dyslexia.
12. Alain, Claude, et al. Left thalamo-cortical
network implicated in successful speech separation and identification. NeuroImage.
June 2005, Vol. 26, 2, pp. 592 – 599.
13. Boets, Bart, et al. Auditory processing,
speech perception and phonological ability in pre-school children at high-risk
for dyslexia: A longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory. Neuropsychologia.
2007b, Vol. 45, 8, pp. 1608–1620.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T0D-4MV0MFF-3/2/51e9753742ca0d8d71470f3766c71b01.
14. Bogliotti, C., et al. Discrimination of
speech sounds by children with dyslexia: Comparisons with chronological age and
reading level controls. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 10
2008, Vol. 101, 2, pp. 137–155. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WJ9-4SFHM3T-1/2/19e6f99059d7b5885fda543c09c5a4ec.
15. Brunellière, Angèle, et al.
Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for the impact of regional
variation on phoneme perception. Cognition. 6 2009, Vol. 111, 3, pp.
390-396. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T24-4VYW6CW-1/2/50c64c08f0f9fa8a5606b5046f43c61c.
16. Gerrits, Ellen and de Bree, Elise. Early
language development of children at familial risk of dyslexia: Speech
perception and production. Journal of Communication Disorders. 5-6 2009,
Vol. 42, 3, pp. 180-194.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T85-4TX33N5-2/2/d3e697145f96b986dbc0a883d92588db.
17. Serniclaes, Willy, et al. Allophonic mode
of speech perception in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 4
2004, Vol. 87, 4, pp. 336-361.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WJ9-4C0SV2F-1/2/2e3ea13d0c54d96d7e1b1cd7b8e7503c.
18. Snowling, Margaret J. The Science of
Dyslexia: A Review of Contemporary Approaches. [book auth.] Martin Turner and
John Rack. The Study of Dyslexia.
19. Facoetti, Andrea and Molteni, Massimo. The
gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2001,
Vol. 39, 4, pp. 352–357. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T0D-427JW9G-3/2/9fb1d063dd119f86522f3185aa7aa0b2.
20. Mycroft, Rachel H., Behrmann, Marlene and Kay,
Janice. Visuoperceptual deficits in letter-by-letter reading? Neuropsychologia.
6 2009, Vol. 47, 7, pp. 1733-1744.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T0D-4VM446F-1/2/b8091c90b6dc1487722d4330a7d13fc4.
21. Lorusso, M. L., et al. Wider recognition in
peripheral vision common to different subtypes of dyslexia. Vision Research.
9 2004, Vol. 44, 20, pp. 2413–2424.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T0W-4CN9KBH-1/2/54f66aaf5254c77c8272aa111061ec95.
22. Barber, Horacio A. and
Kutas, Marta. Interplay between computational models and cognitive
electrophysiology in visual word recognition. Brain Research Reviews. 1
2007, Vol. 53, 1, pp. 98-123.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6SYS-4KMYKYH-1/2/370c0ffcd158fd0bbfb5f70abf6b1158.
23. Bartelson, Paul. The onset of literacy:
Liminal remarks. Cognition. 11 1986, Vol. 24, 1-2, pp. 1-30.
http://www.scopus.com/scopus/inward/record.url?eid=2-s2.0-0022814217&partnerID=40.
24. Nicolson,
25. Corbetta, Maurizio M. FMRI studies of
visual motion and attention. Academic Radiology. December 2001, Vol. 8,
12.
26. Mano, Q. R., Osmon, D. C. and Klein, L.
Factor analysis of visuoperceptual-orthographic processing: Speed, Early
Perception, Late–Post Perception, and Morphological Awareness.
27. Baillieux, Hanne, et al. Developmental
dyslexia and widespread activation across the cerebellar hemispheres. Brain
& Language. 2 2009, Vol. 108, 2, pp. 122–132.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WC0-4TVFY8V-1/2/a8a72a7d9d3afa13ea2e01e0f4abca29.
28. Schulz, Enrico, et al. Impaired semantic
processing during sentence reading in children with dyslexia: Combined fMRI and
ERP evidence. NeuroImage. 5 15, 2008, Vol. 41, 1, pp. 153–168. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WNP-4S1C8C4-4/2/4b28de16b9052de70c8ed0823e3c9021.
29. Steinbrink, C., et al. The contribution of
white and gray matter differences to developmental dyslexia: Insights from DTI
and VBM at 3.0 T. Neuropsychologia. 11 2008, Vol. 46, 13, pp. 3170-3178.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T0D-4T2M664-2/2/29586e03ad83d01f90eb058ed828ead8.
30. Rae, Caroline, et al. Cerebellar morphology
in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2002, Vol. 40, 8, pp.
1285-1292. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T0D-44KVSNH-1/2/bec8e5db3bfadc304e27c82f4f1d008f.
Table 1
Different types of errors in
writing of text according to gender and education level |
||||||
|
|
Different
types of errors in writing |
|
|
||
|
N |
Acoustic
errors |
Visual
errors |
Linguistic
errors |
χ2 |
p |
Boys |
54 |
816 |
324 |
451 |
6.61 |
.038 |
Girls |
54 |
432 |
172 |
301 |
|
|
Totals |
108 |
1248 |
496 |
752 |
|
|
2nd grade |
60 |
665 |
279 |
432 |
1.81 |
.405 |
3rd grade |
60 |
644 |
242 |
390 |
|
|
Totals |
120 |
1299 |
521 |
822 |
|
|
Table 2
Different types of
errors in writing of text according to children’s reading ability,
handwriting skills and deficits in compliance with the linear constraints |
||||||
|
|
Different types of errors in writing |
|
|
||
|
N |
Acoustic errors |
Visual errors |
Linguistic errors |
χ2 |
p |
With
RP |
50 |
856 |
295 |
470 |
11.60 |
.003 |
With
not RP |
50 |
366 |
181 |
251 |
|
|
Totals |
100 |
1222 |
476 |
721 |
|
|
With
bad HW |
50 |
603 |
669 |
79 |
10.58 |
.005 |
With
good HW |
50 |
172 |
240 |
43 |
|
|
Totals |
100 |
775 |
909 |
122 |
|
|
Deficits
in LRS |
49 |
797 |
283 |
430 |
15.18 |
.001 |
No
deficits |
49 |
298 |
142 |
238 |
|
|
Totals |
98 |
1095 |
425 |
668 |
|
|
Table 3
Means and Standard
Deviations Comparing Children’s Problems in Reading |
||||||||||||||||
|
Acoustic errors |
Visual errors |
Linguistic errors |
|||||||||||||
|
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
|||||||
Have
reading problems |
43 |
8.35 |
7.515 |
22 |
2.91 |
2.266 |
44 |
13.91 |
10.811 |
|||||||
Have
not reading problems |
61 |
5.84 |
4.716 |
32 |
1.59 |
1.012 |
68 |
7.81 |
7.095 |
|||||||
Total |
104 |
6.88 |
6.126 |
54 |
2.13 |
1.749 |
112 |
10.21 |
9.199 |
|||||||
One-way Analysis of
Variance Summary table Comparing Reading Problems on AE, VE and LE |
||||||||||||||||
Source |
df |
SS |
MS |
F |
p |
|||||||||||
Acoustic
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
159.247 |
159.247 |
4.383 |
.039 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
102 |
3706.128 |
36.335 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
103 |
3865.375 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Visual
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
22.556 |
22.556 |
8.406 |
.005 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
52 |
139.537 |
2.683 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
53 |
162.093 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Linguistic
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
994.126 |
994.126 |
13.021 |
.000 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
110 |
8398.151 |
76.347 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
111 |
9392.277 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Table 4
Means and Standard Deviations
Comparing Children’s Reading problems (major or minor) |
||||||||||||||||
|
Acoustic
errors |
Visual
errors |
Linguistic
errors |
|||||||||||||
|
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
|||||||
Reading problems
(major) |
31 |
7.45 |
6.490 |
19 |
2.84 |
2.243 |
34 |
10.35 |
8.811 |
|||||||
|
20 |
5.70 |
5.983 |
10 |
1.90 |
1.524 |
19 |
8.37 |
10.068 |
|||||||
No reading problems |
27 |
5.70 |
4.340 |
10 |
1.50 |
.707 |
29 |
10.10 |
8.112 |
|||||||
Total |
104 |
6.88 |
6.126 |
53 |
2.17 |
1.740 |
110 |
10.39 |
9.177 |
|||||||
One-way Analysis of Variance
Summary table Comparing Reading Problems (major or minor) on AE, VE and LE |
||||||||||||||||
Source |
df |
SS |
MS |
F |
p |
|||||||||||
Acoustic errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between Groups |
2 |
185.785 |
92.892 |
2.550 |
.083 |
|||||||||||
Within Groups |
101 |
3679.590 |
36.432 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
103 |
3865.375 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Visual errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between Groups |
2 |
23.326 |
11.663 |
4.347 |
.018 |
|||||||||||
Within Groups |
50 |
134.145 |
2.683 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
52 |
157.472 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Linguistic errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between Groups |
2 |
903.276 |
451.638 |
5.839 |
.004 |
|||||||||||
Within Groups |
107 |
8276.915 |
77.354 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
109 |
9180.191 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Table 5
Means and Standard Deviations Comparing Children’s
Handwriting |
||||||||||||||||
|
Acoustic errors |
Visual errors |
Linguistic errors |
|||||||||||||
Handwriting |
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
|||||||
Have
bad handwriting |
75 |
7.57 |
6.531 |
42 |
2.26 |
1.901 |
77 |
11.62 |
9.463 |
|||||||
Have
not bad handwriting |
29 |
5.07 |
4.543 |
11 |
1.82 |
.874 |
33 |
7.52 |
7.874 |
|||||||
Total |
104 |
6.88 |
6.126 |
53 |
2.17 |
1.740 |
110 |
10.39 |
9.177 |
|||||||
One-way Analysis of
Variance Summary table Comparing Children’s Handwriting on AE, VE and LE |
||||||||||||||||
Source |
df |
SS |
MS |
F |
p |
|||||||||||
Acoustic
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
131.166 |
131.166 |
3.583 |
.061 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
102 |
3734.209 |
36.610 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
103 |
3865.375 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Visual
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
1.716 |
1.716 |
.562 |
.457 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
51 |
155.755 |
3.054 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
52 |
157.472 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Linguistic
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
389.871 |
389.871 |
4.790 |
.031 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
108 |
8790.320 |
81.392 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
109 |
9180.191 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Table 6
Means and Standard
Deviations Comparing Children’s LRS |
||||||||||||||||
|
Acoustic errors |
Visual errors |
Linguistic errors |
|||||||||||||
LRS |
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
N |
M |
SD |
|||||||
Deficits |
63 |
7.62 |
6.489 |
35 |
2.23 |
1.911 |
65 |
11.95 |
9.845 |
|||||||
No
deficits |
41 |
5.73 |
5.399 |
18 |
2.06 |
1.392 |
45 |
8.13 |
7.671 |
|||||||
Total |
104 |
6.88 |
6.126 |
53 |
2.17 |
1.740 |
110 |
10.39 |
9.177 |
|||||||
One-way Analysis of
Variance Summary table Comparing Children’s LRS on AE, VE and LE |
||||||||||||||||
Source |
df |
SS |
MS |
F |
p |
|||||||||||
Acoustic
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
88.469 |
88.469 |
2.389 |
.125 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
102 |
3776.906 |
37.028 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
103 |
3865.375 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Visual
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
.356 |
.356 |
.116 |
.735 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
51 |
157.116 |
3.081 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
52 |
157.472 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Linguistic
errors |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Between
Groups |
1 |
388.129 |
388.129 |
4.768 |
.031 |
|||||||||||
Within
Groups |
108 |
8792.062 |
81.408 |
|
|
|||||||||||
Total |
109 |
9180.191 |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
© Copyright-VIPAPHARM. All rights reserved
web hosting and internet marketing by Siteowners Ltd